Why single women tend to not by lonely while single men tend to be lonely

can’t relate

can’t relate

Roped femboy
Joined
Jun 10, 2023
Posts
304
Reputation
330
1) women tend to have a large social capital and social support of friends when single.

2)
Women generally possess an extremely high level of in-group preference while men generally do not possess any (Rudman and Goodwin, 2004; Patterson-Neubert, 2004). In fact, research has shown that society at large holds an anti-male/pro-female bias (Connors et al., 2023; Whitley and Kite, 2009). Graso et al. (2023) found that women are generally very accepting of instrumental harm (IH) befalling men to achieve some purpose, but are not so tolerant of IH befalling women. High in-group preference has been correlated with high out-group derogation, dehumanization of the out-group, out-group homogeneity bias (seeing out-groups as homogeneous) and an acceptance for double standards (Gilovich et al., 2015; Forsyth, 2017).

Although men, historically and cross-culturally, are the dominant sex, they possess remarkably weaker in-group bias than do women. In four experiments, we found this sex difference persisted using both implicit and explicit measures, despite using a gender attitude IAT that was unconfounded with gender stereotypes… Thus, we can claim with confidence that even when men are responding automatically, their in-group bias is surprisingly frail and that women’s in-group bias is particularly strong at the implicit level (i.e., stronger than men’s by a factor of 4.5). (pg.506)



Rudman, L. A., & Goodwin, S. A. (2004). Gender Differences in Automatic In-Group Bias: Why Do Women Like Women More Than Men Like Men? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 87(4), 494–509. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.87.4.494

.

Goodwin, also a member of Purdue's Women Studies Program, teamed with Laurie Rudman, professor of psychology at Rutgers University, to conduct four studies measuring implicit attitudes of 379 adults. Their results show that women prefer women, and men, on average, don't have a preference toward either gender. Although men, on average, did not show in-group favoritism, individual men's responses varied widely, with some men showing a preference for women and others showing a preference for men… The results showed that women showed in-group favoritism four times greater than men. Those figures are based on an average of separate studies that involved research groups of varying sizes.

Patterson-Neubert, A. (2004, December 13). New research: Women like women more than men like men. Purdue.edu. https://www.purdue.edu/uns/html4ever/2004/041213.Goodwin.gender.html

.

Across five studies (N = 5,204), we investigated implicit evaluations of targets varying in race, gender, social class, and age. Overall, the largest and most consistent evaluative bias was pro-women/anti-men bias… An integrative data analysis highlighted a number of moderating factors, but a stable pro-women/anti-men and pro-upper-class/anti-lower-class bias across demographic groups. (pg.22)

Connor, P., Weeks, M., Glaser, J., Chen, S., & Keltner, D. (2023). Intersectional implicit bias: Evidence for asymmetrically compounding bias and the predominance of target gender. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 124(1), 22–48. https://doi.org/10.1037/pspa0000314

.

[T]here is a bias in favor of women that Alice Eagly and Antonio Mladinic (1994) have dubbed the women are wonderful effect. Their work shows that the global category “women” is viewed more positively than the global category “men” on traditional evaluative measures. This finding has been widely replicated and is supported by earlier research on attitudes toward the sexes (Eagly, Mladinic, & Otto, 1991), although, interestingly, the pattern went unnoticed for many years. This pattern also emerges in measures of implicit attitudes toward women and men. Laurie Rudman and Stephanie Goodwin (2004) found that positive words, such as good, happy, and paradise, are associated more often with women than with men, a preference that was particularly strong for women participants. (pg.466-467)



Whitley, B. E., Jr, & Kite, M. E. (2009). The psychology of prejudice and discrimination (2nd ed.). Wadsworth Publishing.

.

Even in these traditionally female contexts, participants still more readily accepted IH to men than women. Findings indicate people (especially women) are less willing to accept instrumental harm befalling women (vs. men). (pg.2433)

Although we predict a greater tolerance for instrumental harm borne by men than by women, not all individuals will espouse such an asymmetry to equal degrees. A substantial body of evidence finds women exhibit stronger in-group biases favoring their own gender than do men (Rudman & Goodwin, 2004), suggesting greater acceptance of IH to men than women will be especially pronounced among women. Across countries, women express stronger hostility toward men and lower hostility toward women (Glick et al., 2004), suggesting if anyone should exhibit the hypothesized gender bias in instrumental harm acceptance, it should be women. Supporting this prediction, laboratory experiments find women redistribute payments to favor low-earning female (but not male) workers, whereas men showed no such gender bias (Cappelen et al., 2019). In the courtroom, women filing workplace discrimination claims were more likely to win compensation when their case was adjudicated by a female judge (Knepper, 2018). These patterns might be explained by a stronger bias in moral typecasting among women, whereby women more easily recognize other women as victims and men as perpetrators of harm (Reynolds et al., 2020). (pg.2434-2435)

The current investigation sought to examine whether people were more willing to endorse interventions when IH was borne by men than women. Our first two studies supported this premise. Importantly, however, our results showed that this asymmetry was driven primarily by women, but not men, being more likely to accept IH to men than to women across a variety of contexts (pg.2441)



Graso, M., Reynolds, T. & Aquino, K. Worth the Risk? Greater Acceptance of Instrumental Harm Befalling Men than Women. Arch Sex Behav 52, 2433–2445 (2023). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10508-023-02571-0

.

One factor that increases the likelihood of dehumanization might sound surprising: loyalty to valued social groups. When we feel strongly committed to a group, whether a political party, an ethnic group, or a sports team, we are more disposed to dehumanize others. In one survey of over 180 groups in developing countries, tribes characterized by powerful ingroup loyalty and a strong sense of “we feeling” were more likely to dehumanize other tribes and act in violent fashion against them (Cohen, Montoya, & Insko, 2006). (pg.502)



Gilovich, T., Keltner, D., Chen, S., & Nisbett, R. E. (2015). Social Psychology. W. W. Norton & Company.

.

The sociologist William Graham Sumner (1906) maintained that humans are, by nature, a species that joins together in groups. But he also noted a second, equally powerful, human tendency: favoring one’s own group over all others. “Each group nourishes its own pride and vanity, boasts itself superior, exalts its own divinities, and looks with contempt on outsiders”

At the group level, this tendency is called the ingroup–outgroup bias… When people succumb to double-standard thinking, they redefine their own group’s actions as generous and just, and condemn the very same actions taken by the outgroup as hostile and unjust.

Outgroup Homogeneity Bias Most group members are quick to point out the many characteristics that distinguish them from the other members of their own group (“Why, I’m not like them at all!”), but when they evaluate members of outgroups, they underestimate their variability (“They all look the same to me”). (pg.458-459)



Forsyth, D. R. (2017). Group Dynamics (7th ed.). CENGAGE Learning Custom Publishing.
 
  • +1
Reactions: Mio, Jnumber01, Abhorrence and 1 other person
Woman ain't happy shut up
 
Whatever makes you sleep at night.
Woman are lonely hence why they're fat and have high standards
Social groups mean fuck all woman hate other woman you cretin
 
Everyone gets lonely. Men are just seen as autistic turbolosers if they don't fuck, though, so that puts more pressure on them. Single women are viewed as a little sad, but it's not the same level of social pressure.
 
  • +1
Reactions: copemaxxeer, Maalik, Abhorrence and 1 other person
study is superfluous. wymxn have more outlets to help them put off worrying about it, with the caveat that it will hit them harder eventually when they realize no eggs and no future. that's why old cat ladies tend to be more deranged than older bachelors
 
  • +1
Reactions: can’t relate
good thread
 
  • +1
Reactions: can’t relate
SPEAKINGF FACTSS MAA NIGGA! SOO WISE

nah seriously aside, you can better delete tha before some weird satanic liberal woman see this and ruin you life
Na idgf give or take take 3 weeks after blepharoplasty maybe I will start caring lol
 
  • Love it
Reactions: DR. NICKGA
Not reading all that jfl
 
Everyone gets lonely. Men are just seen as autistic turbolosers if they don't fuck, though, so that puts more pressure on them. Single women are viewed as a little sad, but it's not the same level of social pressure.
Basically this.

If a man chooses to be single, like I did for 3 months until I met my girlfriend recently, he is seen as an autistic turbo loser. If a woman chooses to be single, its empowering. My girlfriend had been single since June until she met me. My girlfriend feels self-conscious about her age (48, I'm a 38 year old oldcel) and the fact that she can't bear me children. She had menopause at 40. But the reality is most women her age hate any man who isn't a top <5% man.

I personally have never really had a strong inclination towards having kids. I hate wagecucking more than I love kids. So I don't mind that my girlfriend is infertile. In fact it makes sex more convenient because I don't have to worry about wearing condoms or having her go on birth control.
 
  • +1
Reactions: Pikabro
Shouldn't this read "women have high standards, hence why they're fat and lonely"?
They're not lonely though. Women will never be happy with a man who doesn't turn her on and sexually satisfy her. Women would rather be alone than be with a man who doesn't make her pussy wet and doesn't make her cum. Female loneliness is really just them thinking ">tfw no wealthy Chad husband."

I have a recent theory that how much a woman is willing to settle is related to how strong her sex drive is. My girlfriend absolutely loves sex. She is very orgasmic. Vaginally orgasmic. Can cum from penetration alone. Which is not true for lots of women. I think this is why she's willing to overlook my flaws. But the women on r/WomenDatingOverForty and my sister (whose 49), they'll never be satisfied with any man who wants exclusivity with her. All the men that they actually want don't want them for anything more than casual sex.
 
Last edited:
It's over for old white men jfl
 
its way too long, dnd read a single word
 
1) women tend to have a large social capital and social support of friends when single.

2)
Women generally possess an extremely high level of in-group preference while men generally do not possess any (Rudman and Goodwin, 2004; Patterson-Neubert, 2004). In fact, research has shown that society at large holds an anti-male/pro-female bias (Connors et al., 2023; Whitley and Kite, 2009). Graso et al. (2023) found that women are generally very accepting of instrumental harm (IH) befalling men to achieve some purpose, but are not so tolerant of IH befalling women. High in-group preference has been correlated with high out-group derogation, dehumanization of the out-group, out-group homogeneity bias (seeing out-groups as homogeneous) and an acceptance for double standards (Gilovich et al., 2015; Forsyth, 2017).

Although men, historically and cross-culturally, are the dominant sex, they possess remarkably weaker in-group bias than do women. In four experiments, we found this sex difference persisted using both implicit and explicit measures, despite using a gender attitude IAT that was unconfounded with gender stereotypes… Thus, we can claim with confidence that even when men are responding automatically, their in-group bias is surprisingly frail and that women’s in-group bias is particularly strong at the implicit level (i.e., stronger than men’s by a factor of 4.5). (pg.506)



Rudman, L. A., & Goodwin, S. A. (2004). Gender Differences in Automatic In-Group Bias: Why Do Women Like Women More Than Men Like Men? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 87(4), 494–509. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.87.4.494

.

Goodwin, also a member of Purdue's Women Studies Program, teamed with Laurie Rudman, professor of psychology at Rutgers University, to conduct four studies measuring implicit attitudes of 379 adults. Their results show that women prefer women, and men, on average, don't have a preference toward either gender. Although men, on average, did not show in-group favoritism, individual men's responses varied widely, with some men showing a preference for women and others showing a preference for men… The results showed that women showed in-group favoritism four times greater than men. Those figures are based on an average of separate studies that involved research groups of varying sizes.

Patterson-Neubert, A. (2004, December 13). New research: Women like women more than men like men. Purdue.edu. https://www.purdue.edu/uns/html4ever/2004/041213.Goodwin.gender.html

.

Across five studies (N = 5,204), we investigated implicit evaluations of targets varying in race, gender, social class, and age. Overall, the largest and most consistent evaluative bias was pro-women/anti-men bias… An integrative data analysis highlighted a number of moderating factors, but a stable pro-women/anti-men and pro-upper-class/anti-lower-class bias across demographic groups. (pg.22)

Connor, P., Weeks, M., Glaser, J., Chen, S., & Keltner, D. (2023). Intersectional implicit bias: Evidence for asymmetrically compounding bias and the predominance of target gender. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 124(1), 22–48. https://doi.org/10.1037/pspa0000314

.

[T]here is a bias in favor of women that Alice Eagly and Antonio Mladinic (1994) have dubbed the women are wonderful effect. Their work shows that the global category “women” is viewed more positively than the global category “men” on traditional evaluative measures. This finding has been widely replicated and is supported by earlier research on attitudes toward the sexes (Eagly, Mladinic, & Otto, 1991), although, interestingly, the pattern went unnoticed for many years. This pattern also emerges in measures of implicit attitudes toward women and men. Laurie Rudman and Stephanie Goodwin (2004) found that positive words, such as good, happy, and paradise, are associated more often with women than with men, a preference that was particularly strong for women participants. (pg.466-467)



Whitley, B. E., Jr, & Kite, M. E. (2009). The psychology of prejudice and discrimination (2nd ed.). Wadsworth Publishing.

.

Even in these traditionally female contexts, participants still more readily accepted IH to men than women. Findings indicate people (especially women) are less willing to accept instrumental harm befalling women (vs. men). (pg.2433)

Although we predict a greater tolerance for instrumental harm borne by men than by women, not all individuals will espouse such an asymmetry to equal degrees. A substantial body of evidence finds women exhibit stronger in-group biases favoring their own gender than do men (Rudman & Goodwin, 2004), suggesting greater acceptance of IH to men than women will be especially pronounced among women. Across countries, women express stronger hostility toward men and lower hostility toward women (Glick et al., 2004), suggesting if anyone should exhibit the hypothesized gender bias in instrumental harm acceptance, it should be women. Supporting this prediction, laboratory experiments find women redistribute payments to favor low-earning female (but not male) workers, whereas men showed no such gender bias (Cappelen et al., 2019). In the courtroom, women filing workplace discrimination claims were more likely to win compensation when their case was adjudicated by a female judge (Knepper, 2018). These patterns might be explained by a stronger bias in moral typecasting among women, whereby women more easily recognize other women as victims and men as perpetrators of harm (Reynolds et al., 2020). (pg.2434-2435)

The current investigation sought to examine whether people were more willing to endorse interventions when IH was borne by men than women. Our first two studies supported this premise. Importantly, however, our results showed that this asymmetry was driven primarily by women, but not men, being more likely to accept IH to men than to women across a variety of contexts (pg.2441)



Graso, M., Reynolds, T. & Aquino, K. Worth the Risk? Greater Acceptance of Instrumental Harm Befalling Men than Women. Arch Sex Behav 52, 2433–2445 (2023). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10508-023-02571-0

.

One factor that increases the likelihood of dehumanization might sound surprising: loyalty to valued social groups. When we feel strongly committed to a group, whether a political party, an ethnic group, or a sports team, we are more disposed to dehumanize others. In one survey of over 180 groups in developing countries, tribes characterized by powerful ingroup loyalty and a strong sense of “we feeling” were more likely to dehumanize other tribes and act in violent fashion against them (Cohen, Montoya, & Insko, 2006). (pg.502)



Gilovich, T., Keltner, D., Chen, S., & Nisbett, R. E. (2015). Social Psychology. W. W. Norton & Company.

.

The sociologist William Graham Sumner (1906) maintained that humans are, by nature, a species that joins together in groups. But he also noted a second, equally powerful, human tendency: favoring one’s own group over all others. “Each group nourishes its own pride and vanity, boasts itself superior, exalts its own divinities, and looks with contempt on outsiders”

At the group level, this tendency is called the ingroup–outgroup bias… When people succumb to double-standard thinking, they redefine their own group’s actions as generous and just, and condemn the very same actions taken by the outgroup as hostile and unjust.

Outgroup Homogeneity Bias Most group members are quick to point out the many characteristics that distinguish them from the other members of their own group (“Why, I’m not like them at all!”), but when they evaluate members of outgroups, they underestimate their variability (“They all look the same to me”). (pg.458-459)



Forsyth, D. R. (2017). Group Dynamics (7th ed.). CENGAGE Learning Custom Publishing.

Women lonely = no chad dick for two days
Male lonely = crippling depression and anxiety stemming from constant societal rejection
 
  • +1
Reactions: Mio

Similar threads

6"4 Tyrone(I'm not)
Replies
12
Views
536
Sexually Disabled
Sexually Disabled
John Cracovizk
Replies
22
Views
754
Runawaytictac
Runawaytictac
nevermind2
Replies
12
Views
596
nevermind2
nevermind2
Xangsane
Replies
211
Views
3K
Xangsane
Xangsane

Users who are viewing this thread

Back
Top